An Introduction to the Study of Language
[chapter]
Leonard Bloomfield
1983
Amsterdam Studies in the Theory and History of Linguistic Science : Series 2, Classics in Psycholinguistics
Tliis little book is intended, as tlie title implies, for the general reader and for the student who is entering upon linguistic work. Jts purpose is the same, accordingly, as that of Whitney's Language and the Study of Language and TJie Life and Grouih of Language, books which fifty years ago represented the attainments of linguistic science and, owing to their author's clearness of view and conscientious discrimination between ascertained fact and mere surmise, contain little to which we
more »
... t today subscribe. The great progress of our science in the last half-century is, I believe, nevertheless sufficient excuse for my attempt to give a summary of what is now known about language. That the general reader needs such information as is here given was recognized by Whitney, who wrote, in the preface of his first-named book: 'It can hardly admit of question that at least so much knowledge of the nature, history, and classifications of language as is here presented ought to be included in every scheme of higher education.' While questions of a linguistic nature are everywhere a frequent subject of discussion, it is surprising how little even educated people are in touch with the scientific study of language. I hope that my book will furnish a simple aid for those who choose to make up this deficiency in our scheme of general education Students whose vocation demands linguistic knowledge are subjected in our universities to a detached course or VI PREFACE two on details of the phonologic and morphologic history of such languages as Old English, Gothic, or Old French, details which are meaningless and soon forgotten, if no instruction as to their concrete significance has preceded. To this method of presentation is due, I think, the dislike which so many workers in related fields bear toward linguistic study. I hope that this essay may help to introduce students of philosophy, psychology, ethnology, philology, and other related subjects to a juster acquaintance with matters of language. In accordance with this twofold aim, I have limited myself to a presentation of the accepted doctrine, not even avoiding well-used standard examples. In a few places I have spoken of views that cannot claim more than probability, of hypotheses, and of problems yet to be solved, but I have done this explicitly and only because I think it fitting to indicate the diiection in which our study is at present tending. Consequently the matter here presented is by no means my own, but rather the property of all students of language. It will be found in fuller form and with bibliographic support in the books mentioned in Chapter Ten, and these books I may therefore name as my more immediate sources.^) It will be apparent, especially, that I depend for my psychology, general and linguistic, entirely on Wundt; I can only hope that I have not misrepresented his doctrine. The day is past when students of mental sciences could draw on their own fancy or on 'popular psychology' for their views of mental occurrence. L. B.
doi:10.1075/cipl.3.03blo
fatcat:lksqtjx66rfdbggjyij2g4nvqy