The Crossmodal Influence of Odor Hedonics on Facial Attractiveness: Behavioural and fMRI Measures [chapter]

Francis McGlone, Robert A., Luisa M., Charles Spence
2013 Functional Brain Mapping and the Endeavor to Understand the Working Brain  
Facial attractiveness is a highly relevant social cue, readily assessed by human observers. Facial attractiveness significantly impact on success in both work and social environments [ , ]. Taking a Darwinian perspective, Perrett at al. [ ] have argued that the physical structure of beautiful faces -as judged by others -provide salient signals of mate value that motivate behavior in others. Several general features have been shown to contribute to the perceived attractiveness of a face,
more » ... g both facial symmetry and the extent to which an individual face conforms to an average prototype [ , , ]. Additionally, faces displaying various emotional expressions e.g., joy, anger, etc. have been used to investigate the brain regions involved in the coding of affect [ , , , , ], such as the orbitofrontal cortex OFC , the insular cortex, and the amygdala. At both an explicit and implicit level, humans through the ages have devised means by which to enhance facial attractiveness a multibillion dollar cosmetic industry attests to this fact . An equally lucrative fragrance industry exploits the hedonic primacy of odors in the human brain, yet it remains unclear whether the presence of odors can modulate the perceived attractiveness of faces. A pioneering positron emission tomography PET study by Nakamura and colleagues [ ] demonstrated that activity in left frontal brain regions correlates with perceived facial attractiveness in humans. Furthermore, functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI has been used to show that the viewing of attractive female faces by male participants activates reward circuitry in the brain, in particular, the nucleus accumbens and the OFC [ , ]. A recent study investigated the neural circuitry involved in the perception of facial attractiveness more directly by presenting participants with faces of varying attractiveness, while they performed a gender discrimination task [ ]. Correlation analysis with ratings of attractiveness for the presented faces revealed a region in the medial orbitofrontal cortex OFC that responded specifically to facial attractiveness. Physical attractiveness is not, however, solely dependent upon the visual aspects of appearance, but is often modulated by other sensory cues. For example, a person s voice has been shown to influence a speaker s perceived attractiveness [ , ]. Similarly, a person s body odor also influences their perceived attractiveness [ ]. The notion that odors can exert an influence over the perception of facial characteristics is also supported by the observation that the perceived masculinity/femininity of faces may be modulated by the presence of human sex hormone-like chemicals [ ]. Additionally, the presence of a malodor can negatively influence the perceived attractiveness of male faces as rated by female observers in a psychophysical judgment task [ ]. Only a few neuroimaging experiments have simultaneously presented odors and faces to participants, but none have directly assessed the impact of odor valence on facial attractiveness. For example, an fMRI study conducted by Gottfried and colleagues [ ] paired faces with either a pleasant, unpleasant, or neutral odor, in an associative learning paradigm. Their results suggest that the brain regions involved in the processing of positive and negative affect, such as OFC, nucleus accumbens, and amygdala, are engaged during the appetitive and aversive learning process. Additionally, those brain areas previously found to participate in low-level odor processing, such as the piriform cortex and the caudal OFC, were also found to play an active role in the transfer of affective value between the olfactory and visual modalities. However, since no measure of the attractiveness of the faces was obtained, it remains unclear whether odor valence actually influenced the participants perception of the faces. Finally, it could be argued that odors do not necessarily modulate facial attractiveness per se, but rather other affective components of interpersonal perception such as, for example, perceived sympathy [ ]. Alternatively, however, the presentation of the odor could also induce a general change in a person s mood or emotional state that might also be expected to alter facial attractiveness. Indeed, the psychological and physiological literature published to date supports the view that visual stimuli can influence olfactory perception while olfactory cues rarely influence visual perception [ , , , ]. It thus appears reasonable to assume that the simultaneous presentation of an odor will not change the visual characteristics of a face as such, but rather will primarily just changes people s affective reaction to it. The main aim of the present study was therefore to investigate, both behaviorally and using fMRI, whether olfactory cues can modulate visual judgments of facial attractiveness. In particular, we investigated whether olfactory cues of differing hedonic value i.e., pleasant vs. unpleasant enhance and/or reduce the perceived attractiveness of male faces to female participants. Additionally, we selected an artificial body odor and a common male fragrance as the olfactory stimuli for their ecological relevance when paired with human faces. We hypothesized that the OFC, in particular, would show differential responses depending on the perceived attractiveness of the stimuli presented, since this brain region Functional Brain Mapping and the Endeavor to Understand the Working Brain 210 is activated by pleasant / unpleasant smells [ , , , , ], and is also known to encode facial attractiveness [ , ]. . Material and methods . . Participants Twenty-one healthy right-handed female volunteers participated in this study mean age years, age range -years . All of the participants were non-smoking, had no history of nasal dysfunction or allergies to odors and each gave written informed consent after having received the written instructions concerning the study. Three participants had to be removed from the data analysis because of excessive head motion during the brain scanning session, and the data from two participants were discarded because they were unable to detect the presence versus absence of the odors at above chance levels % and % correct, respectively . Consequently, the data analysis at the group level included a total of datasets. The study was approved by the Central Oxford Research Ethics Committee C . . . . Stimuli and task Two odorants were used in this study, an artificial body odor Thiol compound and a popular male fragrance. The odors were diluted in ml of dipropylene glycol at concentrations of . % for the body odor and . % for the male fragrance. The olfactory stimuli were delivered with a custom-built, computer-controlled olfactometer at a flow rate of liters/ second, through Teflon tubes placed directly under the participant s nose. The participants were asked to breathe normally through their nose and to refrain from making any unduly strong sniffing movements. Clean medical air was delivered continuously through the olfactometer except during the delivery of the olfactory stimuli. Twenty male faces taken from the standardized database developed by Perrett and his colleagues [ ] were used as the visual stimuli. These faces have previously been rated for attractiveness on a -point rating scale. We used a subset of these faces, consisting of the faces with the highest attractiveness ratings and the faces with the lowest attractiveness ratings. Full screen color images of the faces were generated using a video projector located outside the scanner room and projected onto a translucent screen placed directly outside the bore of the magnet. A mirror fixed on the head coil allowed the participants to view the screen while lying in the scanner. Each of the faces was presented three times, once together with each of the two odors and once in the absence of any odor, resulting in the three conditions face-pleasant odor , faceunpleasant odor and face-no odor . Additionally, each odor was presented times in the absence of any visual stimulus, resulting in a total of trials being presented to each participant. The order of trials was randomized for each participant, with the sole constraint that the same face was never presented consecutively. The Crossmodal Influence of Odor Hedonics on Facial Attractiveness: Behavioural and fMRI Measures http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/56504 211 At the beginning of each trial, the participants were visually cued by the presentation of a fixation cross to breathe in and detect the presence or absence of an odor which was delivered for ms following the onset of the visual cue Figure . The faces were presented for ms starting ms after the onset of the odor stimuli. This lag between visual and olfactory stimuli was chosen on the basis of the results of a pilot study which had established that participants perceived the onset of both stimuli as concurrent when presented at this temporal delay. Figure 1 . Participants were cued visually with a fixation cross that changed color from red to green to breathe-in and detect the presence or absence of an odor. In the trials where a face was presented, it was shown for 1000ms, beginning 2000ms after the cue to breathe in. After a rest period of 5500ms, the participants had to rate the attractiveness of the face on a 5-point rating scale. When odors were presented in the same trial, odor stimulation started 500ms after the onset of the cue to breathe-in and lasted for 2500ms, terminating together with the face. When odors were presented without a face, the cue to breathe in was displayed for 3000ms and the participants had simply to rate the pleasantness of the odor. Following the presentation of the face, the participants rated its attractiveness on a rating scale ranging from = very unattractive to = very attractive with as the neutral point. For the odor only trials, the participants had to rate the pleasantness of the odor on a similar scale ranging from ='very unpleasant' to ='very pleasant'. Behavioral measures relating to odor detection as well as the ratings were collected using a custom build button-box. The E-Prime software [ ] was used to control stimulus presentation and to collect responses from the participants. . . Data acquisition and analysis Both functional and structural MRI images were acquired using a T Sonata Siemens scanner fitted with an -channel head coil Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany based at the University of Oxford Centre for Clinical Magnetic Resonance Research OCMR . For the Functional Brain Mapping and the Endeavor to Understand the Working Brain 212 functional data series, a total of T * weighted echo-planar imaging EPI volumes were taken over a time period of min. Each volume consisted of continuous oblique tilted approximately º upward from the anterior to posterior commissure line, so as to be aligned with the temporal lobes slices of mm thickness with an in-plane resolution of × mm. These imaging parameters allowed us to image the ventral two thirds of the brain until approximately Z-coordinate of + of the Montreal Neurological Institute MNI standard brain space, to include all primary and secondary olfactory areas in the temporal lobes and OFC, as well as the visual cortex. Other imaging parameters were TR= . s, x matrix, FOV × mm, TE= ms and, flip angle = º. After acquisition of the functional volumes, a B field map was acquired using a combined symmetric and asymmetric spin echo sequence. For registration into standard anatomical space, a single whole brain EPI volume slices, TR= s, other imaging parameters as above as well as a high-resolution, whole-brain T weighted morphological scan inversion-recovery fast gradient echo, mm slice thickness, mm× mm in-plane resolution was acquired after the experimental paradigm had been completed. Statistical image analysis of the functional dataset was carried out using the FMRIB Expert Analysis Tool FEAT www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl . The following pre-processing was applied motion correction using MCFLIRT [ ] spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM mm mean-based intensity normalization of all volumes by the same factor non-linear highpass temporal filtering Gaussian-weighted LSF straight line fitting, sigma= s . A general linear model using the conditions Face-No odor/Face-Body odor/Face-Fragrance/Body odor/ Fragrance as explanatory variables was fitted to the time course at each voxel. Statistical analysis for each experimental run was carried out using FMRIB s Improved Linear Model FILM with local autocorrelation correction [ ]. For group analysis, the individual results were registered both to high-resolution anatomical MR images and to the Montreal Neurological Institute MNI standard image. Registration to high resolution and standard images was carried out using FMRIB s Linear Image Registration Tool FLIRT [ ]. Mixed-effects often referred to as random-effects group analysis was carried out using FMRIB s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects FLAME software [ ] with a cluster threshold of Z> . and a cluster significance threshold of p=. corrected for multiple comparisons [ , , ]. . Results . . Behavioral Comparison of the pleasantness ratings when the two odors were presented in isolation confirmed that the body odor was indeed perceived as significantly [t = . p < . ] less pleasant M = . SEM = . than the fragrance M = . SEM = . , as expected. The rating data for facial attractiveness were analyzed using a repeated-measures analysis of The Crossmodal Influence of Odor Hedonics on Facial Attractiveness: Behavioural and fMRI Measures http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/56504 213 variance ANOVA with the factors of facial attractiveness low vs. high and odor condition pleasant, unpleasant, or odorless . As expected, the results revealed a significant main effect of facial attractiveness, [F , = . , p < . ], with participants judging the pre-selected attractive faces as being more attractive M = . SEM = . than those faces pre-selected to be less attractive M = . SEM = . . Crucially, the main effect of odors on ratings of facial attractiveness was also significant [F , = . , p < . ], demonstrating that the odors affected the perceived attractiveness of the male faces to the female participants. Post-hoc comparisons Bonferroni corrected of the odor conditions revealed that participants rated the same faces as being significantly less attractive when presented together with the unpleasant body odor M = . SEM = . than when presented together with the pleasant odor M = . SEM = . p < . , or in the absence of any odor M = . SEM = . p < . see Figure . The analysis of this behavioral data revealed no significant difference in mean facial attractiveness ratings between the pleasant versus odorless conditions, nor any interaction between facial attractiveness and odor pleasantness [F , < n.s.]. Figure 2 . The average attractiveness ratings (n=16 participants) for the same 10 faces a-priori assumed to be of high attractiveness compared to the 10 low attractiveness faces are shown when either presented with an unpleasant body odor, a pleasant fragrance, or in the absence of any specific odor. The difference in ratings between the attractive and unattractive faces was significant (paired t-test, p <.05) in each odor group. Additionally, faces presented together with the body odor were rated as significantly less attractive than those presented with the fragrance or those presented in the absence of any odor (paired t-test, p <.05) for both high and low attractive faces. Functional Brain Mapping and the Endeavor to Understand the Working Brain 214 . . Neuroimaging data . . . Odor valence We first investigated those brain regions involved in encoding odor valence pleasant vs. unpleasant in the absence of any visual stimuli i.e., faces . For this purpose, we computed two contrasts between the unimodal odor presentations, which were [Fragrance > Body odor] and [Body odor > Fragrance]. Results from the first contrasts showed several regions within the OFC to be more strongly activated by the pleasant fragrance as compared to the unpleasant odor see Figure . These were located bilaterally in the medial OFC along the olfactory sulcus x/y/z = / /-z-score = . and x/y/z = -/ /-z-score = . , and in the lateral OFC in the right hemisphere only x/y/z = / /-z-score = . . Additionally, a small cluster was detected in the right inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis x/y/z = / / z-score = . . Figure 3. Group results (n=16 participants) for brain regions showing differences in brain activation relating to the pleasantness of the odors are shown on coronal slices at different y-coordinates in the canonical MNI 152 space. The contrast [Fragrance > BO] is rendered in orange/yellow and the contrast [BO > Fragrance] is rendered in blue. The unpleasant BO elicited stronger responses in the supramarginal gyrus (yellow), thalamus (red), piriform cortex (white), and lateral OFC (green). In contrast, the pleasant fragrance caused stronger activation primarily in medial OFC (turquoise). The right side of each slice corresponds to the right side of the brain. Conversely, within the OFC, the unpleasant odor activated more strongly only in the left lateral orbital gyrus x/y/z = -/ /-z-score = . . However, activation was stronger in the primary sensory olfactory areas in the piriform cortex/amygdaloid area bilaterally x/y/z = / /-zscore = . and x/y/z = -/ / z-score = . and in the frontal operculum x/y/z = / / zscore = . and x/y/z = -/ / z-score = . . Activation differences were also detected bilaterally in the supramarginal gyrus x/y/z = /-/ z-score = . and x/y/z = -/-/ zscore = . and in the right thalamus x/y/z = /-/ z-score = . . . . . Facial attractiveness In order to highlight those brain areas involved in encoding the attractiveness of the face stimuli independently of odors, we compared the responses to faces with high vs. low The Crossmodal Influence of Odor Hedonics on Facial Attractiveness: Behavioural and fMRI Measures http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/56504 215 attractiveness. For these contrasts, no distinction was made between the various odor conditions Fragrance, Body odor and clean air , which were grouped together. The results revealed that irrespective of the odor condition, the presentation of the more attractive faces led to increased BOLD signal amplitude in the medial OFC in both hemispheres x/y/z = / /-zscore = . and x/y/z = -/ /-z-score = . , see Figure . Two other brain regions also showed stronger activation in response to the more attractive faces, namely the left nucleus accumbens x/y/z = -/ /-z-score = . and the hypothalamus x/y/z = /-/-z-score = . . Conversely, the presentation of the unattractive faces resulted in stronger activation bilaterally in the amygdala x/y/z = / /-z-score = . and x/y/z = -/-/-z-score = . as well as in the right pallidum x/y/z = /-/-z-score = . . Interestingly, the unattractive faces also elicited stronger activation in visual areas in the left inferior occipital gyrus x/y/z = -/-/-z-score = . . Figure 4. Group results (n=16 participants) for brain regions showing differences in brain activation relating to the attractiveness of the faces (high vs. low) are shown on coronal slices at different y-coordinates in the canonical MNI 152 space. The contrast [High > Low] is rendered in orange/yellow and the contrast [Low > High] is rendered in blue. Attractive faces preferentially engaged the medial OFC, whereas the less attractive faces led to stronger activation in the amygdala. The right side of each slice corresponds to the right side of the brain. . . . Odor-face interactions The primary interest of the present study was to address the question of whether or not odor hedonics would influence the perceived or implicit attractiveness of male faces to female participants. The behavioral results confirmed that the same set of faces was rated as being significantly more attractive when accompanied by the pleasant odor as compared to the unpleasant odor. We were specifically interested in the brain regions associated with this effect and therefore contrasted those trials in which the faces were presented with the pleasant male fragrance to those where the same faces were presented with the unpleasant body odor. Similar to the unisensory effects of pleasant vs. unpleasant odors and attractive vs. unattractive faces, the face stimuli caused significantly stronger activation in the medial x/y/z = -/ /-z-Functional Brain Mapping and the Endeavor to Understand the Working Brain 216 score = . and lateral x/y/z = -/ /-z-score = . OFC as well as in the ventral striatum x/y/z = -/ /-z-score = . , when they were presented together with the pleasant odor see Figure . A further analysis of the percentage BOLD signal change in the peak activated voxels in the left OFC see Figure revealed positive signal changes only when faces were presented with the fragrance or in the absence of odors. The responses in these regions to all other stimuli were either close to zero or slightly negative. Figure 5 . Group results (n=16 participants) for brain regions showing differences in brain activation when the same set of faces was presented together with the pleasant fragrance compared to the unpleasant body odor. The contrast [Faces + Fragrance > Faces + BO] is rendered in orange/yellow and the contrast [Faces + BO > Faces + Fragrance] is rendered in blue. The presence of the fragrance preferentially engaged the OFC and ventral striatum. Conversely, the unpleasant odor caused stronger activation in the amygdala, insular cortex, and visual cortex. The right side of each slice corresponds to the right side of the brain. Conversely, a different network of brain regions responded more strongly when the faces were presented together with the unpleasant body odor. The presence of the unpleasant odor caused significantly stronger activation in the amygdala x/y/z = /-/-z-score = . and x/y/z = -/-/-z-score = . and anterior insular cortex x/y/z = / / z-score = . and x/y/z = -/ / z-score = . . Furthermore, we observed significantly stronger responses in the thalamus x/y/z = /-/ z-score = . and x/y/z = -/-/ z-score = . and an extensive cluster located at the junction of the rolandic operculum with the superior temporal sulcus x/ y/z = /-/ z-score = . and x/y/z = -/-/-z-score = . . The only differences in frontal brain regions was found in the right medial frontal gyrus x/y/z = / / z-score = . . Interestingly, both visual cortical areas x/y/z = -/-/-z-score = . as well as the cerebellum x/y/z = /-/-z-score = . and x/y/z = -/-/-z-score = . also displayed stronger activation when the faces were accompanied by the unpleasant odor. A further analysis of the percentage BOLD signal change in the peak activated voxels in the amygdala see Figure revealed positive signal changes for all conditions. The Crossmodal Influence of Odor Hedonics on Facial Attractiveness: Behavioural and fMRI Measures http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/56504 217 Figure 7. The average (n=16 participants) percentage BOLD signal change from baseline in the peak voxels in both the left and right amygdala are shown for each of the five experimental conditions. Error bars depict the standard error. Figure 6. The average (n=16 participants) percentage BOLD signal change in the peak voxels in both medial and lateral OFC are shown for each of the five experimental conditions. Error bars depict the standard error. Functional Brain Mapping and the Endeavor to Understand the Working Brain 218 . Discussion The principal aim of the present study was to determine whether specific pleasant vs. unpleasant odors can exert a significant influence on the perception of facial attractiveness. Behavioral results confirmed that the two odors used in this study a male fragrance and a synthetic body odor were indeed perceived as different with respect to their pleasantness. Significantly, when briefly presented with the same set of male faces, the female participants in this study judged the faces accompanied by the pleasant fragrance as more attractive than when the faces were presented with an unpleasant synthetic body odor. This effect was related to a modulatory effect exerted by the unpleasant odor, as no significant difference in facial attractiveness ratings was found between the pleasant odor and a no odor control condition. In agreement with the behavioral data, the pleasant fragrance activated the medial OFC, a region that encodes the reward value of stimuli across a variety of sensory modalities including olfaction [ , , , ]. Conversely, the unpleasant odor activated a different network or brain regions, including the amygdala which has previously been implicated in the processing of aversive stimuli [ , , ]. There is currently some controversy over the role of the amygdala in the processing of odor hedonics, since the established view that this region specifically encodes aversive odors [ , ] has been challenged by several more recent studies. For example, Anderson and colleagues [ ] used the pleasant odor citral which has a lemon smell and the unpleasant odor valeric acid a rancid smell in high and low concentrations so that they could vary the valence and intensity of these stimuli. They found that amygdala activation was associated with odor intensity, but was independent of valence. Conversely, activity in the OFC was associated with odor valence, independent of intensity. In a similar study using a greater number of odors pleasant and unpleasant , Rolls and colleagues [ ] found that ratings of odor intensity were correlated with the magnitude of the BOLD signal in medial olfactory cortical areas including the piriform and anterior entorhinal cortex , but not in the OFC. In contrast, pleasant odors were found to activate a medial region of the OFC, whereas unpleasant odors activated the left lateral OFC, irrespective of odor intensity. Activation of this area have also been reported after monetary losses, unattractiveness in face stimuli, and the presentation of aversive odors [ , , , ]. Since the odors used in the present study were matched with respect to their intensities, this supports the notion that stimulus aversiveness is encoded in the human amygdala. This view is also supported by the results of a recent study demonstrating that the amygdala does not encode odor valence or intensity per se, but rather appears to contain a general representation of the emotional value of a stimulus [ ]. When the activation seen in response to more attractive faces was compared to that seen in response to relatively less attractive faces, it was found that the former engaged a reward circuit consisting of the medial OFC and nucleus accumbens, consistent with previous studies [ , , ]. By contrast, relatively less attractive faces gave rise to stronger activation in the amygdala, a region implicated in the processing of the emotional expression of faces [ , ] particularly when the expression is negative fear, anger . The results presented here suggest The Crossmodal Influence of Odor Hedonics on Facial Attractiveness: Behavioural and fMRI Measures http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/56504 219 that less attractive faces produce a similar neural response to that elicited by faces displaying negative emotions. The main finding to emerge from this study was that the presence of odors with different hedonic characteristics altered the perception of male facial attractiveness in female participants. Under such conditions of bimodal stimulation, we found that when the faces were presented together with a pleasant fragrance, increased BOLD activation was predominantly observed in the OFC and ventral striatum i.e. in the same regions that are engaged when viewing attractive faces . It appears that the positive valence of the odor interacted with the representation itself not unpleasant , with the neuroimaging data providing a more in these regions, leading to an overall positive emotional response to the multisensory combination of stimuli i.e., face plus odor . This observation is consistent with previous studies that have implicated these regions in the processing of facial attractiveness [ , , ] and positively valenced odors [ , , ]. Despite the observation that attractiveness ratings were no different when faces were presented together with the pleasant odor compared to faces presented in the absence of odors, significantly positive BOLD responses within the OFC were only observed in the former condition. This may suggest that even though the pleasant odor did not increase the consciously perceived and reported visual attractiveness of the faces per se, the medial OFC did and here we conjecture that this effect engaged more implicit affective processes that participants were unable to access consciously, but would nonetheless impact on mechanisms underpinning liking. In contrast, when the male faces were presented with the unpleasant body odor, ratings of facial attractiveness were significantly reduced, compared to the pleasant odor and the odorless condition. The presence of the unpleasant odor caused significantly stronger activation in the insular cortex and the amygdala, both of which have previously been implicated in the representation of negative affect [ , , ] and facial unattractiveness [ ]. It thus appears that the aversiveness of the body odor negatively influences the emotional response to a face, leading to a decrease in its perceived attractiveness, an effect that was observed for faces of both high and low attractiveness. Several factors might explain the observation that the body odor influenced the ratings and brain activation more strongly than the fragrance. First, the hedonic difference of the body odor from hedonically neutral was greater than that of the fragrance rating of . for body odor versus. . for fragrance with being neutral , so that a stronger effect might be expected on that basis alone. In conclusion, even though the behavioral response as measured by overt rating of facial attractiveness in the presence of the pleasant odor, or no odor, condition did not influence facial attractiveness, results from the neuroimaging component of the study did show activations in the reward processing areas of OFC and ventral striatum. Reward region activity as evaluated using fMRI did not therefore follow the results of the behavioural task. The latter, we suggest, lacked the sensitivity required to dissociate between the fragrance and clean air itself not unpleasant of facial attractiveness with the neuroimaging data providing a more sensitive measure of affective state. For the unpleasant body odor, our findings support the notion that in the context of facial beauty, unpleasant odors have higher overt emotional salience than pleasant odors. Functional Brain Mapping and the Endeavor to Understand the Working Brain 220
doi:10.5772/56504 fatcat:7wkrc6djvvfsllvqcsqd5ae6vq